---
Well, I'm just back from the Panama Canal Society's 75th reunion and
I haven't read through the rest of the thread, but it case someone
else hasn't already pointed it out to you, it seems you've missed
the point that a non-linear detector, (the human ear, for example)
when presented with two sinusoidal carriers, will pass the two
carrier frequencies through, as outputs, as well as two frequencies
(sidebands) which are the sum and difference of the carriers.
The human auditory system has many components,
some linear and some not. One must consider which
component is in play when considering whether there
is mixing.
As a first order approximation: the cochlea is a continous
array of resonators that separates the frequency components
the incoming signal. That is pretty linear.
Then the system assesses the amplitudes of the
various separated components. Assessing amplitude
is a nonlinear process.
The cochlea's ability to separate frequencies is not perfect.
If two frequency components are too close the
cochlea is not able to separate them before determining
amplitude. In which case intermodulation occurs in
detecting amplitude and thus there is a 'beat'.
If a 300 Hz tone and a 400 Hz tone are coming in then
the cochlea can separate them into independent areas
and assess their amplitudes independently. No beat.
If the two tones are too close, say 400 Hz and 410
Hz the cochlea can't separate them into separate
areas and assesses the amplitude of the sum of the
two tones. Since amplitude detection is nonlinear
there is intermodulation in that case and thus a beat.
To do amplitude detection the ear does something
like take only the positive half of the
signal, or take the positive absolute value of the
signal or square the signal. Then it
averages it (or otherwise lowpass it).
The first step is nonlinear. It produces intermodulation
if there is more than one frequency component
present in the band of interest.
With a single sine wave input the nonlinear
part of the amplitude detection gives a DC term and
various harmonics of the sine wave. The averaging
filter filters out all the harmonics and leaves
the DC 'amplitude' value.
If there are two frequencies close enough that they
both get into the same amplitude detector
then the nonlinearity of amplitude detection
results in intermodulation.
That gives a DC and a sum and a difference
frequency and harmonics. The averaging lets only
the DC and the difference frequency through. The
difference frequency is the beat.
Thus two tones separated sufficiently in frequency
produce no beat. Two tones within 20 Hz or
so produce a difference frequency beat but no
sum frequency tone.
---
No, it doesn't.
Since the response of the ear is non-linear in amplitude it has no
choice _but_ to be a mixer and create sidebands.
Only if the tones are not separated in frequency
by the cochlea first.
What you see on an oscilloscope are the time-varying amplitude
variations caused by the linear vector summation of two signals
walking through each other in time, and what you see on a spectrum
analyzer is the two spectral lines caused by two signals adding, not
mixing. If you want to see what happens when the two signals hit
the ear, run them through a non-linear amp before they get to the
spectrum analyzer and you'll see at least the two original signals
plus their two sidebands.
Actually the nonlinear part is in the amplitude detection
which is present toward the end of the chain in both human
hearing and in spectrum analyzers.
<snip>